Skip to main content Skip to navigation
Anthropology | Luke Premo

Ph.D., University of Arizona
Associate Professor
Evolutionary Anthropology

Research Interests

Agent-based modeling, cultural evolution and diversity, cultural transmission, evolution of altruism, gene-culture coevolution, human evolution, life history, multilevel selection, paleodemography, social networks, spatially explicit models

I encourage prospective graduate students who are interested in these or related topics to contact me via email or by phone.

Current Research

I’m an evolutionary anthropologist who studies Pliocene and Pleistocene hominin behavior and paleodemography.  My dissertation research focused on how the fragmentation of woodland habitat in East Africa during the Late Pliocene may have affected the evolution of altruistic food sharing among Plio-Pleistocene hominins and the formation of Lower Paleolithic archaeological landscapes.  Although still very interested in the evolution of altruism in humans, lately I’ve been addressing other evolutionary anthropological topics.  Most of my recent research contributes to a long-term goal of answering the following question: when did the form of high-fidelity social learning we commonly refer to as cultural transmission appear in human evolutionary history?

Today’s rapidly changing technology and impressive diversity in languages, traditions, and social institutions are underwritten by our uncanny ability to directly transmit information from one person to another. Because humans can teach each other about advances made during their lifetimes, our societies retain—rather than constantly reinvent—useful ideas and solutions to life-threatening problems. This allows for beneficial modifications to be passed on to others, where each improvement then serves as the basis for future incremental innovation. Many researchers agree that the striking, though relatively recent, success of our species is due in part to this so-called “ratchet effect” of cultural transmission, which allows human culture to track environmental change more rapidly and exploit a wider range of ecological niches.

Yet despite the fact that cultural transmission makes possible the “cumulative culture” to which we are accustomed today, with its far-reaching implications for the trajectory of human evolution, we still don’t know when this ability evolved in our lineage. If evolutionary anthropologists are to adequately address when cultural transmission appeared during our evolutionary history, then we must be able to identify archaeological signatures that are uniquely associated with the presence of cultural transmission. Simulation models have already begun to play an important role in building a better understanding of the relationship between cultural transmission and the archaeological record. For instance, my research shows that a range of factors—including biased forms of cultural transmission as well as group extinction and local recolonization—can reduce the diversity of a culturally transmitted trait in a population.

Cultural transmission might be a relatively recent (within the last 300,000 years) addition to the behavioral repertoire of hominins. But even if cultural transmission is more ancient, learning more about its origin will shed light on another topic of interest—the appearance of so-called “modern human behavior.” Although the definition varies to some degree among archaeologists, most descriptions of modern human behavior emphasize the regular use of symbols and more complex technology in addition to faster rates of culture change and a greater degree of regional cultural differentiation. What caused this important shift in hominin behavior?

The “demographic hypothesis” posits that increased technological complexity and greater diversity in culturally transmitted traits might be explained by increases in population size rather than by sudden changes in hominin cognitive ability underwritten by the appearance of new genetic mutations. But demography is about more than just the number of individuals in a population. The way in which a population is structured also can affect the transmission of information between individuals and, thus, potentially, population-level characteristics like cultural diversity and rates of change. I use cultural evolutionary models to investigate how population structure can affect cultural diversity and technological complexity while controlling for population size. This work shows that the nature of the social network that links members of different groups in a structured population—or the “connectedness” among forager groups—can affect population-level properties of interest to archaeologists, even when population size is held constant. Mobility is another important aspect of hunter-gatherer life that is likely to structure cultural transmission within a structured population. Indeed, my studies show that mobility strategies that emphasize both longer logistical trips between a “home base” and the resources that surround it and less frequent residential moves can inhibit interaction between foraging groups, thereby increasing the cultural diversity observed at the level of a structured population even when the number of individuals in the population is held constant.

Many of my recent studies address cultural diversity and rates of cultural change at the level of a population of social learners. But Paleolithic archaeologists study assemblages of artifacts, not populations of social learners. This is a crucial distinction, for the highly time-averaged nature of most Paleolithic archaeological assemblages provides a unique obstacle to studying cultural transmission in the past. My work shows that time-averaging can significantly modify the diversity level of cultural variants in assemblages that accumulate over many generations. As a result, the degree to which stone tools vary in a time-averaged assemblage is not necessarily representative of any of the populations that deposited the tools in the assemblage. This disconnect becomes problematic if one wishes to use the diversity observed in an assemblage of artifacts that accumulated over thousands of years to assess the presence of cultural transmission in a past population.

My cultural evolution research not only sheds light on some of the theoretical and methodological difficulties anthropologists face but also pushes us to think differently about how we attempt to identify the origin of cultural transmission and the appearance of modern human behavior during the Paleolithic. There is still so much left to learn.

 

 

Courses

Graduate

  • ANTH 547 Models and Simulation
  • ANTH 548 Hunters and Gatherers: Past and Present
  • ANTH 562 Evolutionary Method & Theory in Anthropology & Archaeology
  • ANTH 565 Human Evolution

Undergraduate

  • ANTH 101 General Anthropology
  • ANTH 260 Introduction to Biological Anthropology
  • ANTH 465 Human Evolution
  • ANTH 490 Integrative Themes in Anthropology: Anthropology of the Anthropocene

 

 

Representative Publications

(Please see CV for a complete list)

Lin, S. C. and L. S. Premo (2021) Forager mobility and lithic discard probability similarly affect the distance to raw material discard from source. American Antiquity 86:845-863.

Premo, L. S. (2021) Population size limits the coefficient of variation in continuous traits affected by proportional copying error (and why this matters for studying cultural transmission). Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 28:512-534. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-020-09464-9

Premo, L. S. (2020) Reconciling and reassessing the accumulated copying error model’s population-level predictions for continuous cultural traits. American Anthropologist 122:771-783.  DOI: 10.1111/aman.13485

Ranhorn, K. L., J. Pargeter, L. S. Premo, et al. (2020) Investigating the evolution of human social learning through collaborative experimental archaeology. Evolutionary Anthropology 29:53-55.

Premo, L. S. and J. R. Brown (2019) The opportunity cost of walking away in the spatial iterated prisoner’s dilemma. Theoretical Population Biology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tpb.2019.03.004

Miller-Atkins, G. and L. S. Premo (2018) Time-averaging and the spatial scale of regional cultural differentiation in archaeological assemblages. Science & Technology of Archaeological Research. doi:10.1080/20548923.2018.1504490.

Tennie, C., L. S. Premo, D. Braun, and S. P. McPherron (2017) Early stone tools and cultural transmission: Resetting the null hypothesis. Current Anthropology 58:652-672.

Premo, L. S. (2016) Effective population size and the effect of demography on cultural diversity and technological complexity. American Antiquity 81:605-622.

Premo, L. S. and G. B. Tostevin (2016) Cultural transmission on the taskscape: Exploring the effects of taskscape visibility on cultural diversity. PLoS ONE 11(9):e0161766. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766

Tennie, C., D. Braun, L. S. Premo, and S. P. McPherron (2016) The Island Test for Cumulative Culture in the Paleolithic. In The Nature of Culture, edited by M. Haidle, N. Conard, and M. Bolus, pp. 121-133. Springer Press, Berlin.

Premo, L. S. (2015) Mobility and cultural diversity in central-place foragers: Implications for the emergence of modern human behavior. In Learning Strategies and Cultural Evolution during the Palaeolithic, edited by Alex Mesoudi and Kenichi Aoki, pp. 45-65. Springer Press, Tokyo.

Premo, L. S. (2014) Cultural transmission and diversity in time-averaged assemblages. Current Anthropology 55(1):105-114.

Premo, L. S. (2013) What serves as evidence for the presence (or absence) of Pleistocene language? Journal of Anthropological Sciences 91:257-259.

Premo, L. S. (2012) The shift to a predominantly logistical mobility strategy can inhibit rather than enhance forager interaction. Human Ecology 40:647-649.

Premo, L. S. (2012) Hitchhiker’s guide to genetic diversity in socially structured populations. Current Zoology 58:287-297.

Premo, L. S. (2012) Local extinctions, connectedness, and cultural evolution in structured populations. Advances in Complex Systems 15:1150002:1-18.

Premo, L. S. and J. B. Scholnick (2011) The spatial scale of social learning affects cultural diversity. American Antiquity 76:163-176.

Premo, L. S. and S. L. Kuhn (2010) Modeling effects of local extinctions on culture change and diversity in the Paleolithic. PLoS ONE 5(12): e15582.

Premo, L. S. and J-J. Hublin (2009) Culture, population structure, and low genetic diversity in Pleistocene hominins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 106(1):33-37.

 

Contact Information

College Hall 230
509.335.3483
luke.premo@wsu.edu

Curriculum Vitae